Skip to main content

Lund University Publications

LUND UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES

The surprising utility of target drift in natural heading judgements

Li, Li ; Rushton, Simon ; Chen, Rong Rong and Niehorster, Diederick LU orcid (2015) Vision Sciences Society: 15th annual meeting In Journal of Vision 15(12).
Abstract

Gibson (1950) proposed that optic flow provides information about the direction of self-motion (heading) relative to objects in the environment. Llewellyn (1971) pointed out that the change in egocentric direction of an object, "drift", also provides information about whether an observer is passing to the left or right of the object. We compared the precision of heading judgements with flow and drift cues, presented in isolation, and together. With flow alone, observers were quite precise (< 1°), but observers were more precise with drift, and equally precise with drift alone and with both flow and drift. Next we examined how precision changed with display duration (0.2-1.6s). There was evidence of cue-combination at 0.2s but at... (More)

Gibson (1950) proposed that optic flow provides information about the direction of self-motion (heading) relative to objects in the environment. Llewellyn (1971) pointed out that the change in egocentric direction of an object, "drift", also provides information about whether an observer is passing to the left or right of the object. We compared the precision of heading judgements with flow and drift cues, presented in isolation, and together. With flow alone, observers were quite precise (< 1°), but observers were more precise with drift, and equally precise with drift alone and with both flow and drift. Next we examined how precision changed with display duration (0.2-1.6s). There was evidence of cue-combination at 0.2s but at longer durations the precisions for the drift and both cue conditions were similar and lower than for the flow condition. To explore whether flow contributes at all with display durations longer than 0.2s, we replicated the 0.4s condition and also measured reaction times. Lower reaction times when flow was available suggested that flow does contribute. We next checked to see whether poorer precision in the flow condition was due to the fact that the object probe was presented after the stimulus display. We ran an experiment in which we added simulated gaze rotation noise in the range between -1 and +1°/s in the display to perturb the drift signal. This led to a drop in the precision of heading judgements. We then added a simulated gaze rotation at +1 or -1°/s. This led to not only a larger reduction in the precision but also a large systematic difference in the accuracy of heading judgments between the two rotation rates, suggesting drift dominating flow. We conclude that target drift is a surprisingly powerful cue for the perception of naturalistic object-relative heading. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.

(Less)
Please use this url to cite or link to this publication:
author
; ; and
publishing date
type
Contribution to journal
publication status
published
subject
in
Journal of Vision
volume
15
issue
12
article number
410
publisher
Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.
conference name
Vision Sciences Society: 15th annual meeting
conference location
St. Pete Beach, Florida, United States
conference dates
2015-05-15 - 2015-05-20
external identifiers
  • pmid:26326098
  • scopus:84945190168
ISSN
1534-7362
DOI
10.1167/15.12.410
language
English
LU publication?
no
id
f1b08c4e-59cf-4e7e-83bb-3a7183069675
date added to LUP
2016-04-12 10:43:15
date last changed
2024-01-04 01:26:32
@misc{f1b08c4e-59cf-4e7e-83bb-3a7183069675,
  abstract     = {{<p>Gibson (1950) proposed that optic flow provides information about the direction of self-motion (heading) relative to objects in the environment. Llewellyn (1971) pointed out that the change in egocentric direction of an object, "drift", also provides information about whether an observer is passing to the left or right of the object. We compared the precision of heading judgements with flow and drift cues, presented in isolation, and together. With flow alone, observers were quite precise (&amp;lt; 1°), but observers were more precise with drift, and equally precise with drift alone and with both flow and drift. Next we examined how precision changed with display duration (0.2-1.6s). There was evidence of cue-combination at 0.2s but at longer durations the precisions for the drift and both cue conditions were similar and lower than for the flow condition. To explore whether flow contributes at all with display durations longer than 0.2s, we replicated the 0.4s condition and also measured reaction times. Lower reaction times when flow was available suggested that flow does contribute. We next checked to see whether poorer precision in the flow condition was due to the fact that the object probe was presented after the stimulus display. We ran an experiment in which we added simulated gaze rotation noise in the range between -1 and +1°/s in the display to perturb the drift signal. This led to a drop in the precision of heading judgements. We then added a simulated gaze rotation at +1 or -1°/s. This led to not only a larger reduction in the precision but also a large systematic difference in the accuracy of heading judgments between the two rotation rates, suggesting drift dominating flow. We conclude that target drift is a surprisingly powerful cue for the perception of naturalistic object-relative heading. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.</p>}},
  author       = {{Li, Li and Rushton, Simon and Chen, Rong Rong and Niehorster, Diederick}},
  issn         = {{1534-7362}},
  language     = {{eng}},
  note         = {{Conference Abstract}},
  number       = {{12}},
  publisher    = {{Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.}},
  series       = {{Journal of Vision}},
  title        = {{The surprising utility of target drift in natural heading judgements}},
  url          = {{http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.12.410}},
  doi          = {{10.1167/15.12.410}},
  volume       = {{15}},
  year         = {{2015}},
}