Skip to main content

Lund University Publications

LUND UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES

Disentangling the effects of object position and motion on heading judgments in the presence of a moving object

Ni, Long ; Niehorster, Diederick LU orcid and Li, Li (2015) Vision Sciences Society: 15th annual meeting In Journal of Vision 15(12). p.1012-1012
Abstract

Previous research has found that moving objects bias heading perception only when they occlude the focus of expansion (FOE) in the background optic flow, with the direction of bias depending on whether the moving object was approached or at a fixed distance from the moving observer. However, the effect of object motion on heading perception was confounded with object position in previous studies. Here, we disentangled the contributions of object motion and position to heading bias. In each 1s trial, the display simulated forward observer motion at 1 m/s through a 3D random-dot cloud that had a constant depth range of 0.5-2 m and consisted of 50 dots which were always seen in the field of view. In Experiment 1, an opaque square that had... (More)

Previous research has found that moving objects bias heading perception only when they occlude the focus of expansion (FOE) in the background optic flow, with the direction of bias depending on whether the moving object was approached or at a fixed distance from the moving observer. However, the effect of object motion on heading perception was confounded with object position in previous studies. Here, we disentangled the contributions of object motion and position to heading bias. In each 1s trial, the display simulated forward observer motion at 1 m/s through a 3D random-dot cloud that had a constant depth range of 0.5-2 m and consisted of 50 dots which were always seen in the field of view. In Experiment 1, an opaque square that had a fixed position on the screen contained 9 dots that moved uniformly laterally or in random directions at a fixed distance from the observer. In Experiment 2, the dots in the square had the same lateral motion component as in Experiment 1, but simultaneously approached the moving observer at 1 m/s. The distance between the center of the square and the FOE (±5° or ±10°) was constant throughout the trial. We found that heading perception was biased even when the FOE was not occluded and the bias was in the direction of object motion when the object was at a fixed distance from the moving observer and was in the opposite direction when it was approached in depth. In addition, when the object contained random motion (Experiment 1), heading bias was toward the object position. We conclude that occlusion of the FOE is not a prerequisite for moving objects to induce a heading bias, consistent with Royden's (2002) differential motion model, and that the bias can be due to either object motion or position. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.

(Less)
Please use this url to cite or link to this publication:
author
; and
publishing date
type
Contribution to journal
publication status
published
subject
in
Journal of Vision
volume
15
issue
12
pages
1012 - 1012
publisher
Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.
conference name
Vision Sciences Society: 15th annual meeting
conference location
St. Pete Beach, Florida, United States
conference dates
2015-05-15 - 2015-05-20
external identifiers
  • pmid:26326700
ISSN
1534-7362
DOI
10.1167/15.12.1012
language
English
LU publication?
no
id
9b151e05-b537-4c14-a848-5154bab5dd88
date added to LUP
2016-04-12 10:42:27
date last changed
2019-03-08 03:27:36
@misc{9b151e05-b537-4c14-a848-5154bab5dd88,
  abstract     = {{<p>Previous research has found that moving objects bias heading perception only when they occlude the focus of expansion (FOE) in the background optic flow, with the direction of bias depending on whether the moving object was approached or at a fixed distance from the moving observer. However, the effect of object motion on heading perception was confounded with object position in previous studies. Here, we disentangled the contributions of object motion and position to heading bias. In each 1s trial, the display simulated forward observer motion at 1 m/s through a 3D random-dot cloud that had a constant depth range of 0.5-2 m and consisted of 50 dots which were always seen in the field of view. In Experiment 1, an opaque square that had a fixed position on the screen contained 9 dots that moved uniformly laterally or in random directions at a fixed distance from the observer. In Experiment 2, the dots in the square had the same lateral motion component as in Experiment 1, but simultaneously approached the moving observer at 1 m/s. The distance between the center of the square and the FOE (±5° or ±10°) was constant throughout the trial. We found that heading perception was biased even when the FOE was not occluded and the bias was in the direction of object motion when the object was at a fixed distance from the moving observer and was in the opposite direction when it was approached in depth. In addition, when the object contained random motion (Experiment 1), heading bias was toward the object position. We conclude that occlusion of the FOE is not a prerequisite for moving objects to induce a heading bias, consistent with Royden's (2002) differential motion model, and that the bias can be due to either object motion or position. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.</p>}},
  author       = {{Ni, Long and Niehorster, Diederick and Li, Li}},
  issn         = {{1534-7362}},
  language     = {{eng}},
  note         = {{Conference Abstract}},
  number       = {{12}},
  pages        = {{1012--1012}},
  publisher    = {{Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.}},
  series       = {{Journal of Vision}},
  title        = {{Disentangling the effects of object position and motion on heading judgments in the presence of a moving object}},
  url          = {{http://dx.doi.org/10.1167/15.12.1012}},
  doi          = {{10.1167/15.12.1012}},
  volume       = {{15}},
  year         = {{2015}},
}