Skip to main content

Lund University Publications

LUND UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES

Personal pronouns and person perception – Do paired and nonbinary pronouns evoke a normative gender bias?

Renström, Emma LU ; Lindqvist Eneroth, Anna LU ; Klysing, Amanda LU and Sendén, Marie Gustafsson (2023) In British Journal of Psychology p.1-22
Abstract
Research on gender-fair language aims to identify language inclusive to a multitude of individuals, for example, increasing the visibility of women by using paired pronouns (he/she) instead of generic masculine forms (he). However, binary presentations like he/she might come with unwanted side effects and evoke what we label as normative gender bias. A normative gender bias is defined as when words lead to stronger associations with individuals with normative gender expressions than with individuals with non-normative gender expressions, thus contributing to making non-normative individuals invisible. In three experiments, we compared the extent to which the paired pronoun he/she (Swedish and English), the neo-pronouns hen (Swedish), ze... (More)
Research on gender-fair language aims to identify language inclusive to a multitude of individuals, for example, increasing the visibility of women by using paired pronouns (he/she) instead of generic masculine forms (he). However, binary presentations like he/she might come with unwanted side effects and evoke what we label as normative gender bias. A normative gender bias is defined as when words lead to stronger associations with individuals with normative gender expressions than with individuals with non-normative gender expressions, thus contributing to making non-normative individuals invisible. In three experiments, we compared the extent to which the paired pronoun he/she (Swedish and English), the neo-pronouns hen (Swedish), ze (English), and the generic pronoun singular they (English) evoked a normative gender bias. Swedish- (N = 219 and 268) and English- (N = 837, from the UK) speaking participants read about individuals referred to with the paired pronoun he/she or with hen, ze, or they. In Experiment 1 (Swedish), there was no main effect of condition on a normative bias, but in Experiment 2 (Swedish), the paired pronouns he/she evoked normative gender bias while hen did not. In Experiment 3 (English), both ze and singular they evoked normative gender bias, although normative associations were lower in these conditions compared to he/she. Furthermore, the normative bias was lower among participants who had knowledge about the use of ze as a nonbinary pronoun. Finally, neither ze nor they evoked a normative gender bias when their use was explicitly stated to be nonbinary. A potential explanation for why singular they did not generally result in less normative associations, despite almost all participants knowing about it, may include its more common use as a generic pronoun. Taken together, our results suggest that neo-pronouns, but not paired pronouns, have the potential to evoke less normative associations, but that they must be both (1) actively created new words and (2) well-known to language users as nonbinary pronouns. (Less)
Please use this url to cite or link to this publication:
author
; ; and
organization
publishing date
type
Contribution to journal
publication status
epub
subject
keywords
gender expression, gender-fair language, nonbinary identities, nonbinary pronouns, normative gender bias, pronouns, transgender
in
British Journal of Psychology
pages
1 - 22
publisher
Wiley-Blackwell
external identifiers
  • scopus:85177239643
  • pmid:37984412
ISSN
2044-8295
DOI
10.1111/bjop.12686
language
English
LU publication?
yes
id
57859bbe-e604-4cb4-ada3-93178baf9f1b
date added to LUP
2023-11-22 09:03:01
date last changed
2024-03-27 14:47:51
@article{57859bbe-e604-4cb4-ada3-93178baf9f1b,
  abstract     = {{Research on gender-fair language aims to identify language inclusive to a multitude of individuals, for example, increasing the visibility of women by using paired pronouns (he/she) instead of generic masculine forms (he). However, binary presentations like he/she might come with unwanted side effects and evoke what we label as normative gender bias. A normative gender bias is defined as when words lead to stronger associations with individuals with normative gender expressions than with individuals with non-normative gender expressions, thus contributing to making non-normative individuals invisible. In three experiments, we compared the extent to which the paired pronoun he/she (Swedish and English), the neo-pronouns hen (Swedish), ze (English), and the generic pronoun singular they (English) evoked a normative gender bias. Swedish- (N = 219 and 268) and English- (N = 837, from the UK) speaking participants read about individuals referred to with the paired pronoun he/she or with hen, ze, or they. In Experiment 1 (Swedish), there was no main effect of condition on a normative bias, but in Experiment 2 (Swedish), the paired pronouns he/she evoked normative gender bias while hen did not. In Experiment 3 (English), both ze and singular they evoked normative gender bias, although normative associations were lower in these conditions compared to he/she. Furthermore, the normative bias was lower among participants who had knowledge about the use of ze as a nonbinary pronoun. Finally, neither ze nor they evoked a normative gender bias when their use was explicitly stated to be nonbinary. A potential explanation for why singular they did not generally result in less normative associations, despite almost all participants knowing about it, may include its more common use as a generic pronoun. Taken together, our results suggest that neo-pronouns, but not paired pronouns, have the potential to evoke less normative associations, but that they must be both (1) actively created new words and (2) well-known to language users as nonbinary pronouns.}},
  author       = {{Renström, Emma and Lindqvist Eneroth, Anna and Klysing, Amanda and Sendén, Marie Gustafsson}},
  issn         = {{2044-8295}},
  keywords     = {{gender expression; gender-fair language; nonbinary identities; nonbinary pronouns; normative gender bias; pronouns; transgender}},
  language     = {{eng}},
  month        = {{11}},
  pages        = {{1--22}},
  publisher    = {{Wiley-Blackwell}},
  series       = {{British Journal of Psychology}},
  title        = {{Personal pronouns and person perception – Do paired and nonbinary pronouns evoke a normative gender bias?}},
  url          = {{http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12686}},
  doi          = {{10.1111/bjop.12686}},
  year         = {{2023}},
}